Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorBaranova, Julija
dc.contributor.authorEnfield, N.J.
dc.contributor.authorS. Gísladóttir, Rósa
dc.contributor.authorDrew, Paul
dc.contributor.authorHoymann, Gertie
dc.contributor.authorBrown, Penelope
dc.contributor.authorBlythe, Joe
dc.contributor.authorGipper, Sonja
dc.contributor.authorMagyari, Lilla
dc.contributor.authorManrique, Elizabeth
dc.contributor.authorRossi, Giovanni
dc.contributor.authorFloyd, Simeon
dc.contributor.authorDingemanse, Mark
dc.contributor.authorTorreira, Francisco
dc.contributor.authorH. Kendrick, Kobin
dc.contributor.authorDirksmeyer, Tyko
dc.contributor.authorC. Levinson, Stephen
dc.contributor.authorSan Roque, Lila
dc.contributor.authorEnfield, N.J.
dc.contributor.authorDingemanse, Mark
dc.contributor.authorBaranova, Julija
dc.contributor.authorS. Gísladóttir, Rósa
dc.contributor.authorDrew, Paul
dc.contributor.authorHoymann, Gertie
dc.contributor.authorBrown, Penelope
dc.contributor.authorBlythe, Joe
dc.contributor.authorGipper, Sonja
dc.contributor.authorMagyari, Lilla
dc.contributor.authorManrique, Elizabeth
dc.contributor.authorRossi, Giovanni
dc.contributor.authorFloyd, Simeon
dc.contributor.authorTorreira, Francisco
dc.contributor.authorH. Kendrick, Kobin
dc.contributor.authorDirksmeyer, Tyko
dc.contributor.authorC. Levinson, Stephen
dc.contributor.authorSan Roque, Lila
dc.date.accessioned2018-08-08 12:55:24
dc.date.accessioned2020-04-01T13:31:57Z
dc.date.accessioned2017-06-19 23:55
dc.date.accessioned2018-08-08 12:55:24
dc.date.accessioned2020-04-01T13:31:57Z
dc.date.accessioned2017-06-01 23:55:55
dc.date.accessioned2018-08-08 12:55:24
dc.date.accessioned2020-04-01T13:31:57Z
dc.date.available2020-04-01T13:31:57Z
dc.date.issued2013
dc.identifier630828
dc.identifierOCN: 1030822554en_US
dc.identifier.urihttp://library.oapen.org/handle/20.500.12657/31343
dc.description.abstractIntroduction A comparison of conversation in twenty-one languages from around the world reveals commonalities and differences in the way that people do open-class other-initiation of repair (Schegloff, Jefferson, and Sacks, 1977; Drew, 1997). We find that speakers of all of the spoken languages in the sample make use of a primary interjection strategy (in English it is Huh?), where the phonetic form of the interjection is strikingly similar across the languages: a monosyllable featuring an open non-back vowel [a, æ, ə, ʌ], often nasalized, usually with rising intonation and sometimes an [h-] onset. We also find that most of the languages have another strategy for open-class other-initiation of repair, namely the use of a question word (usually “what”). Here we find significantly more variation across the languages. The phonetic form of the question word involved is completely different from language to language: e.g., English [wɑt] versus Cha'palaa [ti] versus Duna [aki]. Furthermore, the grammatical structure in which the repair-initiating question word can or must be expressed varies within and across languages. In this chapter we present data on these two strategies – primary interjections like Huh? and question words like What? – with discussion of possible reasons for the similarities and differences across the languages. We explore some implications for the notion of repair as a system, in the context of research on the typology of language use. The general outline of this chapter is as follows. We first discuss repair as a system across languages and then introduce the focus of the chapter: open-class other-initiation of repair. A discussion of the main findings follows, where we identify two alternative strategies in the data: an interjection strategy (Huh?) and a question word strategy (What?). Formal features and possible motivations are discussed for the interjection strategy and the question word strategy in order. A final section discusses bodily behavior including posture, eyebrow movements and eye gaze, both in spoken languages and in a sign language.
dc.languageEnglish
dc.subject.classificationthema EDItEUR::C Language and Linguisticsen_US
dc.subject.otherlanguages
dc.subject.otherlanguages
dc.subject.otherGlider competition classes
dc.subject.otherIcelandic language
dc.subject.otherInterjection
dc.subject.otherInterrogative word
dc.subject.otherMandarin Chinese
dc.subject.otherPhonetics
dc.subject.otherSign language
dc.subject.otherSpoken language
dc.titleChapter 12 Huh? What? – A first survey in 21 languages
dc.typechapter
oapen.identifier.doi10.26530/OAPEN_630828
oapen.relation.isPublishedBy7607a2d0-47af-490f-9d2a-8c9340266f8a
oapen.relation.isPartOfBook9e3a988b-662e-4d44-beec-7b023f7c46b5
oapen.relation.isFundedBy7292b17b-f01a-4016-94d3-d7fb5ef9fb79
oapen.collectionEuropean Research Council (ERC)
oapen.chapternumber1
oapen.grant.number240853
oapen.grant.acronymHSSLU
oapen.grant.programHuman sociality and systems of language use
oapen.remark.publicRelevant Wikipedia pages: Glider competition classes - https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Glider_competition_classes; Icelandic language - https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Icelandic_language; Interjection - https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Interjection; Interrogative word - https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Interrogative_word; Mandarin Chinese - https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Mandarin_Chinese; Phonetics - https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Phonetics; Sign language - https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sign_language; Spoken language - https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Spoken_language
oapen.identifier.ocn1030822554


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record